Comments
Description
Transcript
Evaluating the Trait Approach
432 Chapter 11 Personality children’s personalities tend to resemble the personalities of their biological parents and siblings more closely than they do those of the families in which they are raised (Plomin et al., 1998). Further research will determine more clearly what aspects of the environment are most important in shaping personality (Turkheimer & Waldron, 2000). So far, the evidence suggests that personality is not influenced very strongly by elements of the shared environment—such as socioeconomic status—that equally affect all children in the same family. However, nonshared environmental influences, at home and elsewhere, appear to be very important in personality development (Harris, 2000; Loehlin, Neiderhiser, & Reiss, 2003). We need to know more about the exact impact on personality development of nonshared environmental factors that may be different for twins and nontwin siblings. ■ What conclusions are most reasonable? Even those researchers, such as Robert Plomin, who support genetic theories of personality caution that we should not replace “simple-minded environmentalism” with the equally incorrect view that personality is almost completely biologically determined (Plomin & Crabbe, 2000). It is pointless to talk about heredity versus environment as causes of personality, because nature and nurture always intertwine to exert joint and simultaneous influences (Dodge, 2004; Johnson, McGue, & Krueger, 2005). With this caution in mind, we would be well advised to draw only tentative conclusions about the origins of personality differences. The evidence available so far suggests that genetic influences do appear to contribute significantly to the differences among people in many personality traits (Plomin & Crabbe, 2000). As noted earlier, however, there is no evidence of a specific gene for any specific personality trait. The genetic contribution to personality most likely comes as genes influence people’s nervous systems and general predispositions toward certain temperaments (Arbelle et al., 2003; Ebstein, 2006; Grigorenko, 2002). Temperamental factors (e.g., emotionality and sociability) then interact with environmental factors, such as family experiences, to produce specific features of personality (Caspi et al., 2005). For example, children who inherit a tendency toward emotionality might play less with other children and withdraw from social interactions and thus tend to fail to learn important social skills (Eisenberg, Fabes, & Murphy, 1995). These experiences and tendencies, in turn, might foster the self-consciousness and shyness seen in introverted personalities. Notice, though, that genetic predispositions toward particular personality characteristics may or may not appear in behavior, depending on whether the environment supports or stifles them. Changes in genetically predisposed traits are not only possible but may actually be quite common as children grow (Cacioppo et al., 2000). So even though there is a strong genetic basis for shyness, many children learn to overcome this tendency and become quite outgoing (Rowe, 1997). In summary, rather than inheriting specific traits, people appear to inherit the behavioral and emotional raw materials out of which their personalities are shaped by the world. Evaluating the Trait Approach The trait approach, and especially the big-five model, tends to dominate contemporary research in personality. Yet there are several problems and weaknesses associated with this approach. For one thing, trait theories seem better at describing people than at understanding them. It is easy to say, for example, that Marilyn is nasty because she has a strong hostility trait; but other factors, such as the way people treat her, could also be responsible. In other words, trait theories say a lot about how people behave, but they don’t always explain why (Mischel, 2004a, 2004b). Nor do trait theories say much about how traits are related to the thoughts and feelings that precede, accompany, and follow