THINKING CRITICALLY Are Personality Traits Inherited
by taratuta
Comments
Transcript
THINKING CRITICALLY Are Personality Traits Inherited
430 FIGURE Chapter 11 Personality 11.3 Eysenck’s Personality Dimensions Emotional According to Eysenck, varying degrees of emotionalityby stability and introversionextraversion combine to produce predictable trait patterns. Which section of the figure do you think best describes your personality traits? How about those of a friend or a relative? Did you find it any easier to place other people’s personalities in a particular section than it was to place your own personality? If so, why do you think that might be? Moody Anxious Rigid Sober Pessimistic Reserved Unsociable Quiet doing 2 learn Touchy Restless Aggressive Excitable Changeable Impulsive Optimistic Active Introverted Extraverted Sociable Outgoing Talkative Responsive Easygoing Lively Carefree Leadership Passive Careful Thoughtful Peaceful Controlled Reliable Even-tempered Calm Stable Source: Eysenck & Rachman (1965). stem from two related systems in the brain. These are called the behavioral approach system and the behavioral inhibition system (Pickering & Gray, 1999). The behavioral approach system, or BAS, is made up of brain regions that affect people’s sensitivity to rewards and their motivation to seek these rewards. The BAS has been called a “go” system because it is responsible for how impulsive or uninhibited a person is. The behavioral inhibition system, or BIS, includes brain areas that affect sensitivity to possible punishment and the motivation to avoid being punished. The BIS is a “stop” system that is responsible for how fearful or inhibited a person is. Gray says that people with an active behavioral approach system tend to experience positive emotions; people with an active behavioral inhibition system are more likely to experience negative ones (Larsen & Buss, 2005). Gray’s theory is now more widely accepted than Eysenck’s theory—primarily because it is supported by what neuroscientists have learned about brain structures and neurotransmitters and how they operate (Avila, 2001; Franken, Muris, & Georgieva, 2006; Larsen & Buss, 2005; Wacker, Chavanon, & Stemmler, 2006). G T H I N K I N G C R I T I C A L LY ray’s approach-inhibition theory is one of several biologically oriented Are Personality Traits explanations of the origins of personality traits (e.g., Zuckerman, 2004). A related Inherited? approach involves exploring the role of genetics in these traits. For example, consider a pair of identical twins who had been separated at five weeks of age and did not meet again for thirty-nine years. Both men drove Chevrolets, chain-smoked the same brand of cigarettes, had divorced a woman named Linda, were remarried to a woman named Betty, had sons named James Allan, had dogs named Toy, enjoyed similar hobbies, and had served as sheriff ’s deputies (Tellegen et al., 1988). 431 The Trait Approach ■ What am I being asked to believe or accept? Cases like this have helped focus the attention of behavioral geneticists on the possibility that some core aspects of personality might be partly, or even largely, inherited (Bouchard, 2004; Ebstein, 2006; Johnson et al., 2004; Krueger, Markon, & Bouchard, 2003; Noblett & Coccaro, 2005). ■ Is there evidence available to support the claim? FAMILY RESEMBLANCE Do children inherit personality traits in the same direct way as they inherit facial features, coloration, and other physical characteristics? Research in behavioral genetics suggests that personality is the joint product of genetically influenced behavioral tendencies and the environmental conditions each child encounters. Some of the evidence for this assertion comes from the many familiar cases in which children seem to “have” their parents’ or grandparents’ bad temper, generosity, or shyness. More systematic studies have also found moderate but significant correlations between children’s personality test scores and those of their parents and siblings (Davis, Luce, & Kraus, 1994; Loehlin, 1992). Even stronger evidence comes from studies conducted around the world that compared identical twins raised together, identical twins raised apart, nonidentical twins raised together, and nonidentical twins raised apart (Grigorenko, 2002). Whether they are raised apart or together, identical twins (who have exactly the same genes) tend to be more alike in personality than nonidentical twins (whose genes are no more similar than those of other siblings). This research also shows that identical twins are more alike than nonidentical twins in general temperament, such as how active, sociable, anxious, and emotional they are (Pickering & Gray, 1999; Borkenau et al., 2002; Wolf et al., 2004). On the basis of such twin studies, behavioral geneticists have concluded that at least 30 percent, and perhaps as much as 60 percent, of the differences among people in terms of personality traits is due to genetic factors (Caspi, Roberts, & Shiner, 2005). ■ Can that evidence be interpreted another way? Family resemblances in personality could reflect inheritance or social influence. So an obvious alternative interpretation of this evidence might be that family similarities come not from common genes but from a common environment. Children learn many rules, skills, and behaviors by watching parents, siblings, and others; perhaps they learn their personalities as well (Funder, 2004). And the fact that nontwin siblings are less alike than twins may well result from what is called nonshared environments (Plomin, 2004). Nonshared factors include, for example, a child’s place in the family birth order, differences in the way parents treat each of their children, and accidents, illnesses, or events that alter a particular child’s life or health (Paulhus, Trapnell, & Chen, 1999). Nontwins are more likely than twins, especially identical twins, to be affected by these nonshared environmental factors. ■ What evidence would help to evaluate the alternatives? One way to evaluate the idea that personality is inherited would be to locate genes that are associated with certain personality characteristics (Ebstein, 2006). Genetic differences have already been tentatively associated with certain behavior disorders, but most behavioral genetics researchers doubt that there are direct links between particular genes and particular personality traits (Caspi et al., 2005; Reif & Lesch, 2003). Another way evaluate the role of genes in personality is to study people in infancy, before the environment has had a chance to exert its influence. If the environment were entirely responsible for personality, newborns should be essentially alike. However, as discussed in the chapter on human development, infants show immediate differences in activity level, sensitivity to the environment, the tendency to cry, and interest in new stimuli (Rothbart & Derryberry, 2002). These differences in temperament suggest biological, and perhaps genetic, influences. To evaluate the relative contributions of nature and nurture beyond infancy, psychologists have examined the personality characteristics of adopted children. If adopted children are more like their biological than their adoptive parents, this suggests the influence of heredity in personality. If they are more like their adoptive families, a strong role for environmental factors in personality is suggested. In actuality, adopted