Comments
Description
Transcript
Optimal Arousal Theory
300 FIGURE Chapter 8 Motivation and Emotion 8.1 Need (biological disturbance) Drive Reduction Theory and Homeostasis The mechanisms of homeostasis, such as the regulation of body temperature or food and water intake, are often compared to thermostats. If the temperature in a house falls below the thermostat setting, the furnace comes on and brings the temperature up to that preset level, achieving homeostasis. When the temperature reaches the preset point, the furnace shuts off. Unbalanced equilibrium Drive (psychological state that provides motivation to satisfy need) Equilibrium restored Behavior that satisfies need and reduces drive your bodily fluids will be disturbed, creating a biological need for water. One consequence of this need is a drive—thirst—that motivates you to find and drink water. After you drink, the need for water is met, so the drive to drink is reduced. In other words, drives push people to satisfy needs, thus reducing the drives that have been created. This cycle is shown in Figure 8.1. Early drive reduction theorists described two types of drives. Primary drives stem from biological needs, such as the need for food or water. People do not have to learn these basic biological needs or the primary drives to satisfy them (Hull, 1951). Other drives, however, are learned through experience. These learned secondary drives motivate us to act as if we have unmet basic needs. For example, as people learn to associate money with the ability to buy things to satisfy primary drives for food, shelter, and so on, having money becomes a secondary drive. Having too little money then motivates many behaviors—from hard work to stealing—to obtain more funds. Optimal Arousal Theory OPTIMAL AROUSAL AND PERSONALITY People whose ideal, or optimal, level of arousal is high are more likely to smoke, drink alcohol, engage in frequent sexual activity, listen to loud music, eat “hot” foods, and do other things that are stimulating, novel, and risky (Farley, 1986; Zuckerman, 1993). Those whose optimal level of arousal is lower tend to take fewer risks and behave in ways that are less stimulating. As discussed in the personality chapter, differences in optimal arousal may help shape other characteristics, such as whether we tend to be introverted or extraverted. Drive reduction theory can account for a wide range of motivated behaviors, but not for all of them. Consider curiosity, for example. Monkeys, dogs, cats, and rats will work hard simply to enter a new environment, especially if it is complex and full of novel objects (Loewenstein, 1994). And most people, too, can’t resist checking out whatever is new and unusual. We go to the mall opening, watch builders work, surf the Internet, and travel the world just to see what there is to see. People also go out of their way to ride roller coasters, skydive, drive race cars, and do countless other things that do not reduce any known drive (Zuckerman, 1996). In fact, these behaviors create an increase in arousal—the body’s general level of activation. Arousal is reflected in heart rate, muscle tension, brain activity, blood pressure, and other bodily systems (Deschaumes et al., 1991; Plutchik & Conte, 1997). It is usually lowest during deep sleep, but arousal can also be lowered by meditation, relaxation techniques, and various depressant drugs. Increases in arousal tend to occur in response to hunger, thirst, stimulant drugs, and, as just mentioned, stimuli that are intense, sudden, new, or unexpected. Because people sometimes try to reduce their arousal and sometimes try to increase it, some psychologists have suggested that motivation is tied to the regulation of arousal. Specifically, optimal arousal theory suggests that we are motivated to behave in ways that keep or restore an ideal, or optimal level, of arousal (Hebb, 1955). Too much arousal can hurt performance, as when test anxiety interferes with some students’ ability to recall what they have studied. Overarousal can also cause athletes to “choke” so badly that they miss an easy catch or a simple shot (Smith et al., 2000;