Comments
Description
Transcript
LongTerm Memory
Percentage of items recalled 216 Chapter 6 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 3 6 9 12 15 18 Recall interval (in seconds) Source: Data from Peterson & Peterson (1959). FIGURE 6.4 Forgetting in Short-Term Memory This graph shows the percentage of items recalled after various intervals during which rehearsal was prevented. Notice that forgetting was virtually complete after a delay of eighteen seconds. Brown-Peterson procedure A method for determining how long unrehearsed information remains in short-term memory. long-term memory (LTM) The stage of memory for which the capacity to store new information is believed to be unlimited. Memory How long does information remain in short-term memory if you don’t keep rehearsing it? John Brown (1958) and Lloyd and Margaret Peterson (1959) devised the BrownPeterson procedure to measure the duration of short-term memory when no rehearsal is allowed. In this procedure, participants are presented with a group of three letters, such as GRB. They then count backward by threes from some number until they get a signal. Counting prevents the participants from rehearsing the letters. At the signal, they stop counting and try to recall the letters. By varying the number of seconds spent counting backward, the experimenter can determine how much forgetting takes place over time. As you can see in Figure 6.4, information in short-term memory is forgotten rapidly: After only eighteen seconds, participants can remember almost nothing. Evidence from other such experiments also suggests that unrehearsed information can be held in short-term memory for no more than about eighteen seconds. However, if the information is rehearsed or processed further in some other way, it may be encoded into long-term memory. Long-Term Memory When people talk about memory, they are usually referring to long-term memory. Long-term memory (LTM) is the part of the memory system whose encoding and storage capabilities can produce memories that last a lifetime. Encoding in Long-Term Memory Some information is encoded into long-term memory even if we make no conscious effort to memorize it (Ellis, 1991). However, putting information into long-term memory is often the result of more elaborate and conscious processing that usually involves semantic coding. As we mentioned earlier, semantic encoding often leaves out details in favor of the more general, underlying meaning of the information. In a classic study, Jacqueline Sachs (1967) demonstrated the dominance of semantic encoding in long-term memory. Her participants first listened to tape recordings of people speaking. She then showed them sets of similar sentences and asked them to say which contained the exact wording heard on the tape. Participants did well at this task when tested immediately, using mainly short-term memory. However, after twentyseven seconds, they couldn’t be sure which of two sentences they had heard. For example, they could not remember whether they had heard “He sent a letter about it to Galileo, the great Italian scientist” or “A letter about it was sent to Galileo, the great Italian scientist.” They didn’t do as well after the delay because they had to recall information from long-term memory, where they had encoded the general meaning of what they had heard, but not the exact wording. Perhaps you are thinking, “So what?” After all, the two sentences mean the same thing. Unfortunately, when people encode the general meaning of information they hear or read, they can make mistakes about the details (Brewer, 1977). This can be a problem when recalling exact words is important—such as in the courtroom, during business negotiations, and in discussions between students and teachers about previous agreements. Later in this chapter we show that such mistakes occur partly because people encode into long-term memory not only the general meaning of information but also what they think and assume about that information (McDermott & Chan, 2006). Counterfeiters depend on the fact that people encode the general meaning of visual stimuli rather than specific details. For example, look at Figure 6.5, and find the correct drawing of a U.S. penny (Nickerson & Adams, 1979). Research shows that most people from the United States are unsuccessful at this task. People from other countries do poorly at recognizing their country’s coins, too (Jones, 1990). This research has prompted the U.S. Treasury to begin using more distinctive drawings on the paper currency it distributes. The capacity of long-term memory is extremely large. In fact, many psychologists believe that it is literally unlimited (Matlin, 1998). There is no way to prove this, but we do know that people store vast Storage Capacity of Long-Term Memory 217 Storing New Memories WE TR U doing 2 learn by G T OD WE TR U N I LIBERTY Which is the correct image of a U.S. penny? (See p. 218 for the answer.) S LIBERTY (A) S G T OD WE TR U N LIBERTY (B ) S G T OD WE TR U N S T I OD I G N I FIGURE 6 .5 Encoding into Long-Term Memory LIBERTY (C ) (D) (E) Source: Nickerson & Adams (1979). quantities of information in long-term memory that can be remembered remarkably well after long periods of time. For example, people are amazingly accurate at recognizing the faces of their high school classmates after having not seen them for over twenty-five years (Bruck, Cavanagh, & Ceci, 1991). They also do surprisingly well on tests of a foreign language or algebra fifty years after having formally studied these subjects (Bahrick & Hall, 1991; Bahrick et al., 1994). But long-term memories are also subject to distortion. In one study illustrating this point, students were asked to describe where they were and what they were doing at the moment they heard about the verdict in the O. J. Simpson murder trial (Schmolck, Buffalo, & Squire, 2000). The students first reported their recollections three days after the verdict and then again after either fifteen or thirty-two months. Only half the recollections reported at fifteen months were accurate, and 11 percent contained major errors or distortions. Among those reporting after thirty-two months, 71 percent of their recollections were inaccurate, and just over 40 percent contained major errors or distortions. For example, three days after the verdict, a student said he heard about it while in a campus lounge with many other students around him. Thirty-two months later, this same person recalled hearing the news in the living room of his home with his father and sister. Amazingly, most of the students whose memories had been greatly distorted over time were unaware of the distortion; they were very confident that the reports were accurate. Similar findings have been reported in relation to people’s memories of the September 11, 2001, terrorist attacks on New York and Washington, D.C. Even when these memories changed as time went by, people remained confident in their accuracy (Talarico & Rubin, 2003). Later, we see that such overconfidence can also appear in courtroom testimony by eyewitnesses to crime. doing 2 learn A REMARKABLE MEMORY Using only his long-term memby ory, Franco Magnani created amazingly accurate paintings of his hometown in Italy even though he had not seen it for more than thirty years (Sacks, 1992). People like Magnani display eidetic imagery, commonly called photographic memory. About 5 percent of school-age children have eidetic imagery, but it is extremely rare in adults (Haber, 1979). You can test yourself for eidetic imagery by drawing a detailed picture or map of a place that you know well but have not seen recently. How did you do? Magnani's painting Photo of the same scene