Comments
Description
Transcript
Basic Memory Processes
209 The Nature of Memory FIGURE 6.1 Basic Memory Processes Remembering something requires, first, that the information be encoded—put in a form that can be placed in memory. It must then be stored and, finally, retrieved, or recovered. If any of these processes fails, forgetting will occur. Encoding Code and put into memory Types of memory codes • Acoustic • Visual • Semantic Storage Maintain in memory Types of longterm memory • Episodic • Procedural • Semantic Retrieval Recover from memory Types of retrieval • Recall • Recognition times as much information as can be found in all the volumes of the Encyclopaedia Britannica (Hunt, 1982). Keep in mind, however, that although we retain a great deal of information, we also lose a great deal (Wixted, 2004). Consider Tatiana Cooley. She was the U.S. National Memory Champion for three years in a row, but she confesses that she is so absent-minded that she relies on Post-it Notes to remember everyday errands (Schacter, 2001). Obviously, our memory is made up of many different abilities, some of which may be better than others from person to person and from time to time. Memory plays a critical role in your life. Without it, you wouldn’t know how to shut off your alarm, take a shower, get dressed, recognize objects, or communicate. You would be unaware of your own likes and dislikes. You would have no idea of who you are (Craik et al., 1999). The impressive capacity of human memory depends on the operation of a complex mental system (Schacter, 1999). Basic Memory Processes encoding The process of putting information into a form that the memory system can accept and use. acoustic codes Mental representations of stimuli as sounds. visual codes Mental representations of stimuli as pictures. semantic codes Mental representations of experiences by their general meaning. storage The process of maintaining information in the memory system over time. In February 2002, prison warden James Smith lost his set of master keys to the Westville Correctional Facility. As a result, 2,559 inmates were kept under partial lockdown for eight days while the Indiana Department of Correction spent $53,000 to change locks in the affected areas. As it turned out, the warden had put the keys in his pocket when he went home, forgot he had done so, and reported the keys “missing” when they were not in their usual place in his office the next day (Associated Press, 2002). What went wrong? There are several possibilities. Memory depends on three basic processes: encoding, storage, and retrieval (see Figure 6.1). Our absent-minded warden might have had problems with any one of these processes. First, information must be put into memory, a step that requires encoding. Encoding is a process that puts information to be remembered into a form that our memory system can accept and use. We use memory codes to translate information from the senses into mental representations of that information. Acoustic codes represent information as sequences of sounds, such as a tune or a rhyme. Visual codes represent information as pictures, such as the image of your best friend’s face. Semantic codes represent the general meaning of an experience. So if you see a billboard that reads “Huey’s Going-Out-of-Business Sale,” you might encode the sound of the words as if they had been spoken (acoustic coding), the image of the letters as they were arranged on the sign (visual coding), or the fact that you recently saw an ad for Huey’s (semantic coding). The type of coding we use influences what we remember. Semantic coding might allow you to remember the fact that an unfamiliar car was parked in your neighbors’ driveway just before their house was robbed. If little or no other coding took place, however, you might not be able to remember the make, model, or color of the car. The second basic memory process is storage. Storage refers to the holding of information in your memory over time. When you recall a vacation you took in childhood