Comments
Description
Transcript
ShortTerm Memory and Working Memory
214 Chapter 6 Memory eyes fixate at one point for about one-fourth of a second and then rapidly jump to a new position. You perceive smooth motion because you hold the scene in your visual sensory register until your eyes fixate again. Similarly, when you listen to someone speak, your auditory sensory register allows you to experience a smooth flow of information, even though there are actually short silences between or within words. The fact that sensory memories fade quickly if they are not processed further is actually an adaptive characteristic of the memory system (Baddeley, 1998). You simply cannot deal with all of the sights, sounds, odors, tastes, and touch sensations that come to your sense organs at any given moment. Using selective attention, you focus your mental resources on only part of the stimuli around you, thus controlling what information is processed further in short-term memory. Short-Term Memory and Working Memory The sensory registers allow your memory system to develop a representation of a stimulus. However, they can’t perform the more thorough analysis needed if the information is going to be used in some way. That function is accomplished by short-term memory and working memory. Short-term memory (STM) is the part of your memory system that stores limited amounts of information for up to about eighteen seconds. When you check the newspaper or an on-screen guide for the channel number of a TV show you want to watch and then keep that number in mind as you switch channels, you are using shortterm memory. Working memory is the part of the memory system that allows us to mentally work with, or manipulate, the information being held in short-term memory. When you mentally calculate what time you have to leave home in order to have lunch on campus, return a library book, and still get to class on time, you are using working memory. Short-term memory is actually a component of working memory, and together these memory systems allow us to do many kinds of mental work (Baddeley, 2003; Engle & Oransky, 1999). Suppose you are buying something for eighty-three cents. You go through your change and pick out two quarters, two dimes, two nickels, and three pennies. To do this you use both short-term memory and working memory to remember the price, retrieve the rules of addition from long-term memory, and keep a running count of how much change you have so far. Now try to recall how many windows there learn are on the front of the house or apartment where you grew up. In answerby this question, you probably formed a mental image of the building. You doing ing used one kind of working-memory process to form that image, and then you maintained the image in short-term memory while you “worked” on it by counting the windows. So working memory has at least two components: maintenance (holding information in short-term memory) and manipulation (working on that information). 2 selective attention The process of focusing mental resources on only part of the stimulus field. short-term memory (STM) A stage of memory in which information normally lasts less than twenty seconds; a component of working memory. working memory Memory that allows us to mentally work with, or manipulate, information being held in shortterm memory. Encoding in Short-Term Memory The encoding of information in short-term memory is much more elaborate and varied than encoding in the sensory registers (Brandimonte, Hitch, & Bishop, 1992). Acoustic coding (by sound) seems to dominate. This conclusion comes from research on the mistakes people make when encoding information in short-term memory. These mistakes tend to involve the substitution of similar sounds. For instance, Robert Conrad (1964) showed people strings of letters and asked them to repeat the letters immediately. Among their most common mistakes was the replacement of the correct letter with another that sounded like it. So if the correct letter was C, it was often replaced with a D, P, or T. The participants made these mistakes even though the letters were presented visually, without any sound. Studies in several cultures have also shown that items are more difficult to remember if they sound similar. For example, native English speakers do less well when they try to remember a string of letters like ECVTGB (which all have similar sounds) than when trying to remember one like KRLDQS (in which there are different sounds). Encoding in short-term memory is not always acoustic, however. Information in short-term memory also can be encoded visually, semantically, and even kinesthetically 215 Storing New Memories 9 8 3 6 0 1 4 2 8 9 2 6 7 2 4 9 8 5 5 1 5 4 6 7 0 2 6 3 1 8 2 5 4 1 2 8 1 2 5 4 1 4 3 5 9 9 4 8 7 5 4 7 6 6 3 3 3 1 1 9 0 3 7 9 4 2 6 8 4 5 0 2 9 3 7 Source: Howard (1983). FIGURE 6.3 The Capacity of Short-Term Memory Here is a test of your immediate memory span (Howard, 1983). Ask someone to read to you the numbers in the top row at the rate of about one per second; then try to repeat them back in the same order. Then try the next row, and the one after that, until you make a mistake. Your immediate memory span is the maximum number of items you can repeat back perfectly. doing 2 learn by (in terms of physical movements; Best, 1999). In one study, deaf people were shown a list of words and then asked to immediately write down as many as they could remember (Shand, 1982). When these participants made errors, they wrote words that are expressed through similar hand movements in American Sign Language, rather than words that sounded similar to the correct words. Apparently, these individuals had encoded the words on the basis of the movements they would use when making the signs for them. How much information can you hold in short-term memory? The simple test presented in Figure 6.3 will help you determine your immediate memory span, which is the largest number of items you can recall perfectly after one presentation. If your memory span is like most people’s, you can repeat six or seven items from the test in this figure. And you should come up with about the same result whether you use digits, letters, words, or virtually anything else (Pollack, 1953). George Miller (1956) noticed that many studies using a variety of tasks showed the same limit on the ability to process information. This “magic number,” which is seven plus or minus two, appears to be the immediate memory span or capacity of short-term memory, at least in laboratory settings. In addition, the “magic number” refers not only to discrete elements, such as words or digits, but also to meaningful groupings of information, called chunks. To see the difference between discrete elements and chunks, read the learn following letters to a friend, pausing at each dash: FB-IAO-LM-TVQ-VCBby doing MW. The chances are very good that your friend will not be able to repeat this string of letters perfectly. Why? There are fifteen letters, which exceeds most people’s immediate memory span. Now, give your friend the test again, but group the letters like this: FBI-AOL-MTV-QVC-BMW. Your friend will probably repeat the string easily. Although the same fifteen letters are involved, they will be processed as only five meaningful chunks of information. Storage Capacity of Short-Term Memory 2 Chunks of information can be quite complex. If you heard someone say, “The boy in the red shirt kicked his mother in the shin,” you could probably repeat the sentence perfectly. Yet, it contains twelve words and forty-three letters. How can you repeat the sentence so effortlessly? The answer is that you are able to build bigger and bigger chunks of information (Ericsson & Staszewski, 1989). In this case, you might represent “the boy in the red shirt” as one chunk of information rather than as six words or nineteen letters. Similarly, “kicked his mother” and “in the shin” represent separate chunks of information. Learning to use bigger and bigger chunks of information can improve short-term memory. In fact, children’s short-term memories improve partly because they gradually become able to hold as many as seven chunks in memory and also because they get better at grouping information into chunks (Servan-Schreiber & Anderson, 1990). Adults also can greatly increase the capacity of their short-term memory by using more efficient chunking. For example, after extensive training, one college student increased his immediate memory span from seven to eighty digits (Neisser, 2000a). So although the capacity of short-term memory is more or less constant (from five to nine chunks of meaningful information), the size of those chunks can vary tremendously. The Power of Chunking immediate memory span The maximum number of items a person can recall perfectly after one presentation of the items. chunks Stimuli that are perceived as units or meaningful groupings of information. Duration of Short-Term Memory Why don’t you remember every phone number you ever called or every conversation you ever had? The answer is that unless you do something to retain it, information in short-term memory is soon forgotten. This feature of short-term memory is adaptive because it gets rid of a lot of useless information, but it can also be inconvenient. You may have discovered this if you ever looked up a phone number, got distracted before you could call it, and then forgot the number.