Problems with a cognitive model of eating behaviour
by taratuta
Comments
Transcript
Problems with a cognitive model of eating behaviour
Page 145 Black blue EATING BEHAVIOUR 145 contradictory attitudes towards foods in terms of ‘tasty’, ‘healthy’, ‘fattening’ and ‘a treat’. Sparks et al. (2001) incorporated the concept of ambivalence into the Theory of Planned Behaviour and assessed whether it predicted meat or chocolate consumption. Participants were 325 volunteers who completed a questionnaire including a measure of ambivalence assessed in terms of the mean of both positive and negative evaluations (e.g. ‘how positive is chocolate’ and ‘how negative is chocolate’) and then subtracting this mean from the absolute difference between the two evaluations (i.e. ‘total positive minus total negative’). This computation provides a score which reflects the balance between positive and negative feelings. In line with previous TPB studies, the results showed that attitudes per se were the best predictor of the intention to consume both meat and chocolate. The results also showed that the relationship between attitude and intention was weaker in those participants with higher ambivalence. This implies that holding both positive and negative attitudes to a food makes it less likely that the overall attitude will be translated into an intention to eat it. A cognitive approach to eating behaviour, however, has been criticized for its focus on individual level variables only and for the assumption that the same set of cognitions are relevant to all individuals. For example Resnicow et al. (1997) carried out a large scale study involving 1398 school children as a means to predict their fruit and vegetable intake. The study measured social cognitive variables including self-efficacy, social norms and added additional cognitive variables including preferences and outcome expectations. The results showed that only preferences and outcome expectations predicted actual eating behaviour but that 90 per cent of the variance in eating behaviour remained unaccounted for. The authors concluded from this study that ‘SCT (social cognition theory) may not be a robust framework for explaining dietary behaviour in children’ (Resnicow et al. 1997: 275) and suggested that a broader model which included factors such as self esteem, parental and family dietary habits and the availability for fruit and vegetables may be more effective. Problems with a cognitive model of eating behaviour A cognitive model of eating behaviour highlights the role of cognitions and makes explicit the cognitions which remain only implicit within a developmental perspective. It provides a useful framework for studying these cognitions and highlights their impact upon behaviour. However, there are some problems with this approach as follows: I Most research carried out within a cognitive perspective uses quantitative methods and devises questionnaires based upon existing models. This approach means that the cognitions being examined are chosen by the researcher rather than offered by the person being researched. It is possible that many important cognitions are missed which are central to understanding eating behaviour. I Although focusing on cognitions those incorporated by the models are limited and ignore the wealth of meanings associated with food and body size. I Research from a cognitive perspective assumes that behaviour is a consequence of rational thought and ignores the role of affect. Emotions such as fear (of weight Page 145 Black blue