Comments
Description
Transcript
Social Identity Theory
542 Chapter 14 Social Psychology ■ What did the researchers find? Self-reports and GSR measures revealed that participants in all three experiments were significantly less upset by an anxiety-provoking experience (the death film or the threat of shock) if they had first received esteem-building feedback about their previous test performance. ■ What do the results mean? The researchers concluded that their results support the notion that self-esteem can act as a buffer against anxiety and other negative feelings. This conclusion would help explain why people are so eager to maintain or enhance their self-esteem (Tesser, 2001): We don’t like to feel anxious, and increased self-esteem reduces most people’s anxiety. ■ What do we still need to know? These results certainly support terror management theory, but by themselves, they are not broad enough to confirm all of its assumptions. For example, the theory also predicts that when people are sensitized to the threat of death, they will seek to protect themselves by suppressing thoughts of death and also by doing things that increase the approval and support of others in the society in which they live. Consistent with this prediction, people have been found to make larger contributions to charity after they have been made more aware of their own mortality (Jonas et al., 2002). Similarly, dramatic increases in volunteering for charity work occurred after the events of September 11, 2001 (Penner, Dovidio, et al., 2005). But which strategies are people most likely to use, and why? Are some strategies more or less likely to be adopted at different times in a person’s life or among people in certain cultures? And what forms of self-esteem are most important in different cultures? So far, most of the research on terror management theory has been done in individualistic cultures such as North America, where self-esteem is largely based on personal accomplishments. However, terror management theory has also been supported by preliminary studies in Japan, aboriginal Australia, and other collectivist cultures in which feelings of self-worth tend to be more closely tied to the performance and status of the groups to which people belong (Halloran & Kashima, 2004; Heine, Harihara, & Niiya, 2002). It will take many more experiments to test all the predictions derived from the theory. Social Identity Theory Stop reading for a moment, and complete the following sentence: I am a(n) . Some people fill in the blank using characteristics such as “hard doing worker,” “good sport,” or some other aspect of their personal identity. However, many others identify themselves using a word or phrase that refers to their nationality, gender, or religion (Lee & Yoo, 2004). These latter responses reflect social identity, our beliefs about the groups to which we belong. Our social identity is therefore a part of our self-concept (Tropp & Wright, 2001; Vignoles et al., 2006). Our social or group identity permits us to feel part of a larger whole (Ashmore et al., 2004). Its importance is seen in the pride that people feel when a member of their family graduates from college or when a local team wins a big game (Burris, Branscombe, & Klar, 1997). In wars between national, ethnic, or religious groups, individuals sacrifice and sometimes die for the sake of their group identity. A group identity is also one reason people donate money to those in need, support friends in a crisis, and display other helping behaviors. As we shall see later, however, defining ourselves in terms of a group identity can foster an “us versus them” mentality that sets the stage for prejudice, discrimination, intergroup conflict, and even terrorism (Brewer & Pierce, 2005). 2 learn by social identity The beliefs we hold about the groups to which we belong.