Comments
Description
Transcript
Culture Experience and Perception
123 Recognizing the Perceptual World indicating a place to eat. Similarly, perhaps you remember a time when an obviously incompetent referee incorrectly called a penalty on your favorite sports team. You knew the call was wrong because you clearly saw the other team’s player at fault. But suppose you had been cheering for that other team. The chances are good that you would have seen the referee’s call as the right one. Top-Down and Bottom-Up Processing Together doing 2 learn by WHAT DOES IT LOOK LIKE TO YOU? Some people see the Devil’s face in this photo of smoke from the World Trade Center under attack on September 11, 2001. This perception results partly from top-down processes, such as knowledge about the evil of the attack, that make it easier to see something demonic. People who don’t expect to see a face in the smoke—or whose cultural background doesn’t include the “devil” concept—may not see one until that interpretation is suggested. To check that possibility, show this photo to people from various religions and cultures (cover the caption). Ask if they see anything in the smoke, but don’t immediately tell them what to look for. How many, and which ones, identified a demonic face before you mentioned it? FIGURE 3.27 Interaction of Top-Down and Bottom-Up Processing Which obscured line do you find easier to read: the one on the top or the one on the bottom? Top-down processing should help you read the obscured text on the top line. However, in the bottom line, the words are not related, so top-down processing cannot operate. doing 2 learn by Bottom-up and top-down processing usually work together to help us recognize the perceptual world. This interaction is beautifully illustrated by the process of reading. When the quality of the raw stimulus on the page is poor, as in Figure 3.27, top-down processes compensate to make continued reading possible. They allow you to fill in where words are not well perceived and processed, thus giving you a general idea of the meaning of the text. You can fill in the gaps because the world is redundant; it provides multiple clues about what is going on. So even if you lose or miss one stimulus in a pattern, other clues can help you recognize the pattern. There is so much redundancy in written language, for instance, that many of the words and letters you see are not needed. Fo- exmp-e, y-u c-n r-ad -hi- se-te-ce -it- ev-ry -hi-d l-tt-r m-ss-ng. Similarly, vision in three dimensions normally provides multiple cues to depth, making recognition of distance easy and clear. It is when many of these cues are eliminated that ambiguous stimuli create the sorts of depth illusions discussed earlier. In hearing, too, top-down processing can compensate for ambiguous stimuli. In one experiment, participants heard strings of five words in meaningless order, such as “wet brought who socks some.” There was so much background noise, however, that only about 75 percent of the words could be recognized (Miller, Heise, & Lichten, 1951). The words were then read to a second group of participants in a meaningful order (e.g., “who brought some wet socks”). The second group was able to recognize almost all of the words, even under the same noisy conditions. In fact, it took twice as much noise to reduce their performance to the level of the first group. Why? When the words were in meaningless order, only bottom-up processing was available. Recognizing one word was no help in identifying the next. Meaningful sentences, however, provided a more familiar context and allowed for some top-down processing. Hearing one word helped the listener make a reasonable guess (based on knowledge and experience) about the others. (See “In Review: Mechanisms of Pattern Recognition” for a summary of bottomup and top-down processing.) Culture, Experience, and Perception We have been talking as if all aspects of perception work or fail in the same way for everyone, everywhere. The truth is, though, that virtually all perceptual abilities are shaped to some extent by the sensory experiences we have or have not had (Kitayama et al., 2003). For example, people are better at judging the size and distance of familiar objects than of unfamiliar ones. Size and shape constancy, too, depend partly on the knowledge and experience that tell us that most solid objects do not suddenly change their size or shape. The experience-based nature of perception can also be seen in brightness constancy: You perceive charcoal to be darker than a sheet of writing paper partly because, no matter how much light the charcoal reflects, you know charcoal is black. Experience even teaches us when to ignore certain stimulus cues (Yang & Kubovy, 1999). To fully experience the depth portrayed in a painting, for example, you have to ignore ridges, scratches, dust, or other texture cues from the canvas that would remind you of its flatness. And the next in review 124 Chapter 3 Sensation and Perception MECHANISMS OF PAT TERN RECOGNITION Mechanism Description Example Bottom-up processing Raw sensations from the eye or the ear are analyzed into basic features, such as edges, color, or movement; these features are then recombined at higher brain centers, where they are compared with stored information about objects or sounds. You recognize a dog as a dog because its features—four legs, barking, panting—match your perceptual category for “dog.” Top-down processing Knowledge of the world and experience in perceiving allow people to make inferences about the identity of stimuli, even when the quality of raw sensory information is low. On a dark night, a small, vaguely seen blob pulling on the end of a leash is recognized as a dog because the stimulus occurs at a location where we would expect a dog to be. ? 1. Your ability to read a battered old sign that has some letters missing is a result of processing. 2. When stimulus features match the stimuli we are looking for, takes place. 3. Schemas can create a that makes us more likely to perceive stimuli in a particular way. Source: Hudson (1960). FIGURE 3.28 Culture and Depth Cues People in various cultures were shown drawings like these and asked to judge which animal is closer to the hunter. Those in cultures that provide lots of experience with pictured depth cues choose the antelope, which is at the same distance from the viewer as the hunter. Those in cultures less familiar with such cues may choose the elephant, which, though closer on the page, is more distant when depth cues are considered. time you are watching TV, notice the reflections of objects in the room that appear on the screen. You have learned to ignore these reflections, so it will take a little effort to perceive them and a lot of effort to focus on them for long. What if you hadn’t had a chance to learn or practice these perceptual skills? One way to explore this question is through case studies of people who had been blind for decades and then had surgery that restored their sight. It turns out that these people can immediately recognize simple objects and perceive movement, but they usually have problems with other aspects of perception (Gregory, 2005). For example, M.M. had been blind from early childhood. When his vision was restored in his forties, he adjusted well overall, but he still has difficulty with depth perception and object recognition (Fine et al., 2003). Often, as people move toward or away from him, they appear to shrink or inflate. Identifying common objects can be difficult for him, and faces pose a particular challenge. To recognize individuals, he depends on features such as hair length or eyebrow shape. M.M. has trouble, too, distinguishing male faces from female ones and great difficulty recognizing the meaning of facial expressions. He is also unable to experience many of the perceptual illusions shown in this chapter, such as the closure illusion in Figure 3.20(D) or the size illusions in Figure 3.24. For the rest of us, too, the ability to experience perceptual illusions depends on our sensory history. So people who grow up in significantly different sensory environments are likely to have noticeably different perceptual experiences. For example, the size illusion shown in Figure 3.24(A) is strongest in the “carpentered world,” where seeing straight lines is an everyday experience (Leibowitz et al., 1969). Responses to illusions such as this one are not as strong for people from rural Africa and other places in which the visual environment contains more irregular and curved lines than straight ones (Coren & Girgus, 1978). Similarly, responses to depth cues in pictures and paintings differ in cultures that do and do not use such images to represent reality. People in the Me’n or the Nupa cultures of Africa, who have little experience with pictorial representation, have a more difficult time judging distances shown in pictures than do people in picture-oriented cultures (see Figure 3.28). These individuals also tend to have a harder time sorting pictures of threedimensional objects into categories, even though they can easily sort the objects themselves (Derogowski, 1989). And residents of dense tropical rain forests, where most objects are seen over relatively short distances, may have some difficulty when asked to judge the distance of remote objects on an open plain (Turnbull, 1961). In other words, although the structure and principles of human perceptual systems tend to create generally similar views of the world for all of us, our perception of