Comments
Description
Transcript
Surveys Looking at the Big Picture
27 Research Methods in Psychology doing 2 learn by TRANSLATING OBSER VATIONS INTO EVIDENCE Observing peo- ple in natural settings can provide important clues to understanding social interaction and other aspects of behavior and mental processes. It is harder than it looks. Imagine you are studying these children at play, and make a list of the exact behaviors you would count as “aggressive,” “shy,” “fearful,” “cooperative,” and “competitive.” to lift off his wife’s head as if it were a hat and put it on his own head. He could not name common objects, but he could describe them. When handed a glove, for example, he said, “A continuous surface, infolded on itself. It appears to have . . . five outpouchings, if this is the word . . . a container of some sort.” Only later, when he put it on his hand, did he exclaim, “My God, it’s a glove!” (Sacks, 1985, p. 13). Using case studies such as this one, pioneers in neuropsychology have noted the symptoms suffered by people with particular kinds of brain damage or disease (Banich, 2004). Eventually, neuropsychologists were able to tie specific disorders to certain types of injuries, poisons, and other causes. In Dr. P.’s case, it was probably a large brain tumor that caused his symptoms. Case studies do have their limitations. They may not represent people in general, and they may contain only the evidence a particular researcher considered important (Loftus & Guyer, 2002). Nonetheless, when conducted and used with care, case studies can provide valuable raw material for further research and can serve as the testing ground for new treatments, training programs, and other applications of research (Tavris, 2004). Surveys: Looking at the Big Picture surveys Research that involves giving people questionnaires or interviews designed to describe their attitudes, beliefs, opinions, and intentions. In contrast to the individual close-ups provided by case studies, surveys offer wideangle views of large groups. In surveys, researchers use interviews or questionnaires to ask people about their behavior, attitudes, beliefs, opinions, or intentions. Just as politicians and advertisers rely on opinion polls to test the popularity of policies or products, psychologists use surveys to gather descriptive data on just about any behavior or mental process, from parenting practices to sexual behavior. However, the validity of survey data depends partly on the way the survey questions are asked (Bhopal et al., 2004). In one survey study at a health clinic, patients were asked how frequently they experienced headaches, stomachaches, and other symptoms of illness (Schwarz & Scheuring, 1992). If the wording of the question suggested that most people frequently experience such symptoms, the patients said that they frequently experienced them, too. But if the wording suggested that people rarely experience these symptoms, the 28 Chapter 1 Introduction to the Science of Psychology LEARNING FROM RARE CASES Dustin Hoffman’s character in the movie Rain Man was based on the case of “Joseph,” an autistic man who can, for example, mentally multiply or divide sixdigit numbers. Other case studies have described autistic savants who can correctly identify the day of the week for any date in the past or the future or tell at a glance that, say, exactly 125 paper clips are scattered on the floor. By carefully studying such rare cases, cognitive psychologists are learning more about human mental capacities and how they might be maximized in everyone. patients said that they experienced the symptoms infrequently. A survey’s validity also depends on who is surveyed. If the people surveyed do not represent the views of the population you are interested in, it is easy to be misled by survey results (Gosling et al., 2004; Kraut et al., 2004). For example, if you were interested in Americans’ views on how common ethnic prejudice is, you would come to the wrong conclusion if you surveyed only African Americans or only European Americans. To get a complete picture, you would have to survey people from all ethnic groups so that each group’s opinions could be fairly represented. Other limitations of the survey method are more difficult to avoid. For example, the American Society for Microbiology (ASM) found that 95 percent of the U.S. adults it surveyed said that they wash their hands after using toilet facilities. However, naturalistic observations of thousands of people in public restrooms across the United States revealed that the figure is closer to 67 percent (ASM, 2000). In other words, people may be unwilling to admit undesirable or embarrassing things about themselves, or they may say what they believe they should say about an issue. And sometimes those who respond to a survey hold views that differ from those who do not respond (Visser et al., 2000). Survey results—and the conclusions drawn from them—will be distorted to the extent that these tendencies distort people’s responses and researchers’ access to responses (Hoyle et al., 2002). Still, surveys provide an efficient way to gather large amounts of data about people’s attitudes, beliefs, or other characteristics. A SEVERELY FLAWED SUR VEY Using survey methods like this, you could probably get whatever results you want! Psychologists work hard to write questions and use methods that maximize the validity of their surveys’ results. © Scott Adams/Distributed by United Feature Syndicate, Inc.