Learning to Speak Stages of Language Development
by taratuta
Comments
Transcript
Learning to Speak Stages of Language Development
270 Chapter 7 Thought, Language, and Intelligence Long before they say their first words, babies are getting ready to talk. Experiments in Patricia Kuhl’s laboratory show that even six-month-olds tend to look longer at faces whose lip movements match the sounds of spoken words. This tendency reflects babies’ abilities to focus on, recognize, and discriminate the sounds of speech, especially in their native language. These abilities are crucial to the development of language (Mayberry, Lock, & Kazmi, 2002). GETTING READY TO TALK Learning to Speak: Stages of Language Development Children the world over develop language with impressive speed; the average six-yearold already has a vocabulary of about 13,000 words (Pinker, 1994). But acquiring language involves more than just learning vocabulary. We also have to learn how words are combined and how to produce and understand sentences. Psychologists who study the development of language have found that the process begins in the earliest days of a child’s life and follows some predictable steps (Saffran, Senghas, & Trueswell, 2001). The First Year In their first year, infants become more and more attuned to the babblings Repetitions of syllables; the first sounds infants make that resemble speech. sounds that will be important in acquiring their native language. In fact, this early experience with language appears to be vital. Without it, language development can be impaired (Mayberry & Lock, 2003). The first year is also the time when babies begin to produce babblings, which are patterns of meaningless sounds that first resemble speech. These alternating consonant and vowel sounds (such as “bababa,” “dadada,” and “mamimamima”) appear at about four months of age, once the infant has developed the necessary coordination of the tongue and mouth. Though meaningless to the baby, babblings are a delight to parents. Infants everywhere begin with the same set of babbling sounds, but at about nine months of age, they begin to produce only the sounds that occur in the language they hear the most. At about the same time, their babbling becomes more complex and begins to sound like “sentences” in the babies’ native language (Goldstein, King, & West, 2003). Infants who hear English begin to shorten some of their vocalizations to “da,” “duh,” and “ma.” They use these sounds to convey joy, anger, interest, and other messages in specific contexts and with obvious purpose (Blake & de Boysson-Bardies, 1992). By ten to twelve months of age, babies can understand several words—certainly more words than they can say (Fenson et al., 1994). Proper names and object words— such as mama, daddy, cookie, doggy, and car—are among the earliest words they understand. These are also the first words children are likely to say when, at around twelve months of age, they begin to talk (some talk a little earlier and some a little later). Nouns for simple object categories (dog, flower) are acquired before more general nouns (animal, plant) or more specific names (collie, rose; Rosch et al., 1976). Of course, these early words do not sound exactly like adult language. Englishspeaking babies usually reduce them to a shorter, easier form, like “duh” for duck or “mih” for milk. Children make themselves understood, however, by using gestures, voice