...

FOCUS ON RESEARCH Studying EMDR

by taratuta

on
Category: Documents
68

views

Report

Comments

Transcript

FOCUS ON RESEARCH Studying EMDR
32
Chapter 1 Introduction to the Science of Psychology
are allowed to watch: a lot, a little, or none at all. The experimental group is the
group that is exposed to an experience of interest to the experimenter (a lot of violent
TV, for example). A group that receives no such exposure, or a differing amount of
exposure, is called the control group. Control groups provide baselines against which
to compare the performance of the experimental group. If everything about the groups
is exactly the same except for exposure to some experience, then any differences
between groups at the end of the experiment should be due to that experience.
L
FOCUS ON RESEARCH
et’s consider how Francine Shapiro used
the experimental method to explore
Studying EMDR
whether her EMDR treatment was actually causing the improvements she observed in
her clients. (Each chapter to follow contains a Focus on Research section like this one,
which presents a different example of how researchers in psychology ask and answer
questions about behavior and mental processes.)
■ What was the researcher’s question?
Like other scientists, Shapiro phrased her question about the value of EMDR in the
form of a hypothesis, namely that EMDR treatment causes a significant reduction in
anxiety. Does it?
■ How did the researcher answer the question?
To find out, Shapiro operationally defined “EMDR treatment” as making a certain number of back-and-forth eye movements per second for a particular period of time. And
“significant reduction in anxiety” was operationally defined as a certain amount of
reduction in clients’ self-reported discomfort.
Shapiro then identified twenty-two people who were suffering the ill effects of
traumas such as rape or military combat. These were her research participants. As
shown in Figure 1.7, she assigned these participants to two groups. The experimental
group received one fifty-minute session of EMDR. The control group focused on their
unpleasant memories for eight minutes but without moving their eyes back and forth
(Shapiro, 1989b). The experimenter controlled whether EMDR treatment was given to
each participant, so the presence or absence of treatment was the independent variable.
The participants’ anxiety level was the dependent variable. In Shapiro’s experiment,
having a control group allowed her to measure how much change in anxiety might be
expected from exposure to bad memories without EMDR treatment.
■ What did the researcher find?
The results of Shapiro’s experiment showed that participants receiving EMDR treatment experienced a complete and nearly immediate reduction in anxiety related to their
traumatic memories. Those in the control group showed no such change.
■ What do the results mean?
experimental group The group that
receives the experimental treatment.
control group The group that receives
no treatment or provides some other
baseline against which to compare the
performance or response of the experimental group.
At this point, you might be ready to believe that the treatment caused the difference.
Before coming to that conclusion, though, look again at the structure, or design, of the
experiment. The treatment group’s session lasted fifty minutes. The control group
focused on their memories for only eight minutes. Would the people in the control
group have improved if they, too, had spent fifty minutes focusing on their memories?
We don’t know, because the experiment did not compare methods of equal length.
■ What do we still need to know?
Experiments have now been conducted that introduce enough control into the treatment
situation to evaluate alternative explanations for the EMDR effect. Most of the controlled
33
Research Methods in Psychology
1. Preliminary screening
of participants
2. Random assignment
to conditions
Participants are
interviewed by
the researcher,
and their baseline
anxiety is
established.
FIGURE
3. Treatment phase
4. Posttreatment
phase
Experimental
group
Receives EMDR
treatment
Reports on anxiety
Control group
No EMDR
Reports on anxiety
1.7
A Simple Two-Group Experiment
Ideally, the only difference between the
experimental and control group in experiments such as this one is whether the participants receive the treatment that the
experimenter wants to evaluate. Under
these ideal circumstances, at the end of
the experiment any difference in the two
groups’ reported levels of anxiety (the dependent variable) should be attributable
to whether or not they received treatment
(the independent variable).
confounding variable Any factor that
affects the dependent variable along
with, or instead of, the independent
variable.
random variables Uncontrolled or uncontrollable factors that affect the dependent variable along with, or instead
of, the independent variable.
random assignment A procedure
through which random variables are
evenly distributed in an experiment by
placing participants in experimental
and control groups on the basis of a
coin flip or some other random process.
placebo A treatment that contains no
active ingredient but produces an effect
because the person receiving it believes
it will.
studies completed so far cast doubt on whether the eye movements in EMDR are causing
clients’ improvement (e.g., Davidson & Parker, 2001; Lazarus & Lazarus, 2002; Van
Deusen, 2004), but we still don’t know for sure what is producing the effects that EMDR
proponents report. As the research continues, it will be important for these proponents
to avoid confirmation bias, the human tendency to look only for evidence that supports
their own hypotheses. In fact, all scientists have a responsibility to combat confirmation
bias by looking for evidence for and against even their most cherished hypotheses.
Shapiro’s (1989b) experiment reminds us to be on the lookout for flaws in experimental design and control that might affect the conclusions we can draw from research
results. In fact, we have to consider anything that could confuse, or confound, our interpretation of those results. Any factor that might have affected the dependent variable,
along with or instead of the independent variable, may operate as a confounding variable. When confounding variables are present, we can’t tell whether it was the independent variable or the confounding variable that produced the results. Let’s consider
three sources of confounding variables: random variables, participants’ expectations,
and experimenter bias.
Random Variables In an ideal research world, everything about experimental and
control groups would be the same except for their exposure to the independent variable
(such as whether or not they received treatment). In the real world, however, there are
always other differences between the groups that reflect random variables. Random variables are uncontrolled, sometimes uncontrollable, factors, such as differences in the time
of year when research takes place and differences in each participant’s cultural background, personality, health, and sensitivity to stress. There are so many ways in which
participants might vary from each other that it is usually impossible to form groups that
are matched on all of them. Instead, experimenters simply flip a coin or use some other
random process to assign each research participant to experimental or control groups.
These procedures, called random assignment, tend to spread the effects of uncontrolled
variables randomly (and probably about evenly) across groups. This minimizes the chance
that they will distort the results of the experiment (Shadish, Cook, & Campbell, 2002).
Differences in what participants think about the experimental situation can act as another confounding variable.
For example, if participants expect that a treatment will help them, they may try harder
to improve than those in a control group who receive no treatment or a less impressive one. Improvement created by a participant’s expectations is called the placebo effect.
A placebo (pronounced “plah-SEE-boe”) contains nothing known to be helpful, but
it still produces benefits because a person believes it will do so (Stewart-Williams &
Podd, 2004; Wager et al., 2004).
Participants’ Expectations: The Placebo Effect
Answer key to Table 1.7: The independent variable (IDV) in experiment 1 is the type of reading class; the
dependent variable (DV) is reading skill. In experiment 2, the IDV is the quality of sleep; the DV is the score
on a memory test. In experiment 3, the IDV is amount of exercise; the DV is lung capacity. In experiment 4,
the IDV is using or not using a cell phone; the DV is performance on a simulated driving task.
34
Chapter 1 Introduction to the Science of Psychology
EVER SINCE I STARTED WEARING
THESE MAGNETS . . . Placebo-
controlled experiments are vital for
establishing cause-effect relationships
between treatments and outcomes with
human participants. For example, many
people swear that magnets held against
their joints relieve the pain of sports injuries and even arthritis. Some research
supports this view (e.g., Harlow et al.,
2004), but most experiments have found
magnets to be no more effective than
placebo treatment with an identical, but
nonmagnetic, metal object (Collacott et
al., 2000; Feingold & Flamm, 2006;
Winemiller et al., 2003).
How can researchers estimate the strength of placebo effects in an experiment?
The most common strategy is to include in the experimental design a special control group that receives only a placebo. The researchers then compare results for the
experimental group, the placebo group, and a group of participants receiving no
treatment. In one stop-smoking study, for example, participants in a placebo group
took sugar pills that the experimenter said would help them endure the stress of giving up cigarettes (Bernstein, 1970). These participants did as well at quitting as those
in the experimental group, who had received extensive treatment. This result suggested that the experimental group’s success may have been due largely to the participants’ expectations, not to the treatment methods. Some studies suggest the same
conclusion about the effects of EMDR, because significant anxiety reduction has
been observed in clients who get a version of the treatment that does not involve
eye movements or even focusing on traumatic memories (Cahill, Carrigan, & Frueh,
1999; Cusack & Spates, 1999; Rosen, 1999). In fact, although EMDR appears to benefit some clients, its failure to outperform impressive placebo treatments (or other
established methods for helping clients deal with unpleasant images) has led many
researchers to conclude that EMDR should not be a first-choice treatment for anxietyrelated disorders (Davidson & Parker, 2001; Lohr et al., 2003; Taylor, 2004; Taylor
et al., 2003).
Experimenter Bias Another possible confounding variable is experimenter
bias, the unintentional effect that researchers can exert on their own results. Robert
experimenter bias A confounding variable that occurs when an experimenter
unintentionally encourages participants
to respond in a way that supports the
hypothesis.
Rosenthal (1966) was one of the first to demonstrate the power of experimenter bias.
His participants were laboratory assistants who were asked to place rats in a maze.
Rosenthal told some of the assistants that their rats were “maze-bright.” He told the
others that their rats were “maze-dull.” In truth, both groups of rats were randomly
drawn from the same population and had equal maze-learning capabilities. Still, the
so-called maze-bright animals learned the maze significantly faster than the “mazedull” ones. Why? Rosenthal concluded that the result had nothing to do with the rats
and everything to do with the experimenters. He suggested that the assistants’ expectations about their rats’ supposedly superior (or inferior) capabilities caused them to
slightly alter their training and handling techniques. These slight differences may have
speeded (or slowed) the animals’ learning. Similarly, when giving different kinds of
Fly UP