THINKING CRITICALLY Does Day Care Harm the Emotional Development of Infants
by taratuta
Comments
Transcript
THINKING CRITICALLY Does Day Care Harm the Emotional Development of Infants
364 Chapter 9 Human Development leaves, but when she returns the child acts angry and rejects the mother’s efforts at contact; when picked up, the child squirms to get down. If the relationship is disorganized, the infant’s behavior is inconsistent, disturbed, and disturbing; the child may begin to cry again after the mother has returned and provided comfort, or may reach out for the mother while looking away from her (Moss et al., 2004). The nature of a child’s attachment to caregivers can have long-term and far-reaching effects (NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 2006b). For example, unless disrupted by the loss of a parent, abuse by a family member, chronic depression in the mother, or some other severe negative event (Weinfield, Sroufe, & Egeland, 2000), an infant’s secure attachment continues into young adulthood—and probably throughout life (Hamilton, 2000; Mattanah, Hancock, & Brand, 2004; Waters et al., 2000). A secure attachment to the mother is also reflected in relationships with other people. Children who are securely attached receive more positive reactions from other children when they are toddlers (Fagot, 1997) and have better relations with peers in middle childhood and adolescence (Carlson, Sroufe, & Egeland, 2004; Schneider, Atkinson, & Tardif, 2001). Patterns of child care and attachment vary widely in different parts of the world. In northern Germany, for example, where parents are quite strict, the proportion of infants who display avoidant attachments is much higher than in the United States (Spangler, Fremmer-Bombik, & Grossman, 1996). Kibbutz babies in Israel, who sleep in infant houses away from their parents, are relatively likely to show insecure attachments and other related problems (Aviezer et al., 1999). In Japan, where mothers are expected to be completely devoted to their children and are seldom apart from them, even at night, children develop an attachment relationship that emphasizes harmony and union (Rothbaum et al., 2000). These attachment patterns differ from the secure type that is most common in the United States: With their parents’ encouragement, U.S. children balance closeness and proximity with exploration and autonomy. W T H I N K I N G C R I T I C A L LY ith about 60 percent of mothers of infants in the United States workDoes Day Care Harm the ing outside the home, concern has Emotional Development been expressed about how daily separations from their mothers might affect children, of Infants? especially infants (Clarke-Stewart & Allhusen, 2005). Some have argued that putting infants in day care, with a baby sitter or in a day-care center, damages the quality of the motherinfant relationship and increases the babies’ risk for psychological problems later on (Gallagher, 1998). ■ What am I being asked to believe or accept? The claim is that daily separations created by day care damage the formation of an attachment between the mother and infant and harm the infant’s emotional development. ■ Is there evidence available to support the claim? There is clear evidence that separation from the mother is painful for young children. If separation lasts a week or more, children who have formed an attachment to their mother tend to protest, then become apathetic and mournful, and finally seem to lose interest in the missing mother (Robertson & Robertson, 1971). But day care does not involve such lasting separations. Research has shown that infants who are in day care do form attachments to their mothers. In fact, they prefer their mothers to their daytime caregivers (Lamb & Ahnert, 2006). Are these attachments as secure as the attachments formed by infants whose mothers do not work outside the home? Researchers have examined this question by comparing how infants react to brief separations from their mothers in the Strange Situation. A