Comments
Description
Transcript
Moral Reasoning
374 doing 2 learn by Chapter 9 Human Development HANGING OUT, SEPARATELY Ethnic identity is that part of our personal identity that reflects the racial, religious, or cultural group to which we belong. Ethnic identity often leads people to interact mainly with others who share that same identity. To what extent is this true of you? You can get a rough idea by jotting down the ethnicity of all the people you chose to spend time with over the past week or so. independence, but late adolescence is also a time when many people become more aware of their obligations to their families (Fuligni & Pedersen, 2002). By the time they are twenty-one, about half of the adolescents studied have resolved the identity crisis in a way that is consistent with their self-image and the historical era in which they are living. They enter young adulthood with self-confidence. Basically the same people who entered adolescence, they now have more mature attitudes and behavior, more consistent goals and values, and a clearer idea of who they are (Savin-Williams & Demo, 1984). Many have become more aware of their obligations to their families (Fuligni & Pedersen, 2002). For those who fail to resolve identity issues—either because they avoided the identity crisis by accepting the identity their parents set for them or because they postponed dealing with the crisis and remain uncommitted and lacking in direction—there are often problems ahead (Lange & Byrd, 2002). Moral Reasoning Adolescents are able to develop an identity partly because, according to Piaget’s theory, they have entered the formal operational period, which allows them to think logically and reason about abstract concepts. Adolescents often find themselves applying these advanced cognitive skills to questions of morality. To examine how people think about morality, psychologists have asked them how they would resolve moral dilemmas, and why. Perhaps the most famous of these is the “Heinz dilemma,” in which people must decide whether a man named Heinz should steal a rare and unaffordably expensive drug in order to save his wife from cancer. By posing moral dilemmas such as this one, Lawrence Kohlberg found that the reasons given for moral choices change systematically and consistently with age (Kohlberg & Gilligan, 1971). Young children make moral judgments that differ from those of older children, adolescents, or adults. Kohlberg proposed that moral reasoning develops in six stages, which are summarized in Table 9.3. These stages, he said, are not tightly linked to a person’s chronological age. Instead, there is a range of ages for reaching each stage, and not everyone reaches the highest level. Stage 1 and Stage 2 moral judgments, which are most typical of children under the age of nine, tend to be selfish in nature. Kohlberg called this level of moral reasoning preconventional, because it is not based on the conventions or rules that usually guide social interactions in society. People at this level of moral development are mainly concerned with avoiding punishment or following rules when it is to their own advantage. At the conventional level of moral reasoning, Stages 3 and 4, people care about other Kohlberg’s Stages of Moral Reasoning preconventional Referring to moral reasoning that is not based on the conventions or rules that guide social interactions in a society. conventional Referring to moral reasoning that reflects a concern about other people, as well as the belief that morality consists of following rules and conventions. 375 Adolescence TA B L E 9.3 Kohlberg’s Stages of Moral Development Kohlberg’s stages of moral reasoning describe differences in how people think about moral issues. Here are some examples of answers that people at different stages of development might give to the “Heinz dilemma” described in the text. This dilemma is more realistic than you might think. In 1994, a man was arrested for robbing a bank after being turned down for a loan to pay for his wife’s cancer treatments. Stage Preconventional 1 2 Conventional 3 4 Postconventional 5 6 What Is Right? Should Heinz Steal the Drug? Obeying and avoiding punishment from a superior authority Making a fair exchange, a good deal “Heinz should not steal the drug, because he will be jailed.” “Heinz should steal the drug, because his wife will repay him later.” Pleasing others and getting their approval “Heinz should steal the drug, because he loves his wife and because she and the rest of the family will approve.” “Heinz should steal the drug for his wife, because he has a duty to care for her,” or “Heinz should not steal the drug, because stealing is illegal.” Doing your duty, following rules and social order Respecting rules and laws, but recognizing that they may have limits Following universal ethical principles, such as justice, reciprocity, equality, and respect for human life and rights “Heinz should steal the drug, because life is more important than property.” “Heinz should steal the drug because of the principle of preserving and respecting life.” people. They think that morality consists of following rules and conventions such as duty to the family, to marriage vows, and to the country. The moral reasoning of children and adolescents from nine to nineteen is most often at this level. Stages 5 and 6 represent the highest level of moral reasoning, which Kohlberg called postconventional because it occurs after conventional reasoning. Moral judgments at this level are based on personal standards or universal principles of justice, equality, and respect for human life, not just on the demands of authority figures or society. People who have reached this level view rules and laws as arbitrary but respect them because they protect human welfare. They believe that individual rights can sometimes justify violating these laws if the laws become destructive. People do not usually reach this level until sometime after the end of adolescence. Stage 6 is seen only rarely, in extraordinary individuals. Studies of Kohlberg’s stages have generally supported the sequence he proposed (Turiel, 2006). postconventional Referring to moral reasoning that reflects moral judgments based on personal standards or universal principles of justice, equality, and respect for human life. Limitations of Kohlberg’s Stages Do Kohlberg’s stages appear across cultures? In general, yes. Forty-five studies in twenty-seven cultures from Alaska to Zambia showed that people do tend to make upward progress through Kohlberg’s stages, without reversals (Snarey, 1987). Stages 5 and 6, however, did not always appear. Further, the moral judgments made in some cultures do not always fit neatly into Kohlberg’s stages. For example, some people in collectivist cultures, such as Papua New Guinea, Taiwan, and Israeli kibbutzim, explained their answers to moral dilemmas by pointing to the importance of the community. And people in India included in their moral reasoning the importance of acting in accordance with one’s caste (social class) and with