Comments
Description
Transcript
Our Sense of Smell
103 The Chemical Senses: Taste and Smell 1991). Many animals easily learn taste aversions to particular foods when the taste is associated with nausea, but humans learn aversions to odors more readily than to tastes (Bartoshuk & Wolfe, 1990). Variations in the state of our nutrition also affect our experience of taste and flavor, as well as our motivation to eat particular foods. Food deprivation or salt deficiency makes sweet or salty things taste better. Influences on protein and fat intake are less direct. Protein and fat molecules have no particular taste or smell. So preferring or avoiding foods that contain these nutrients is based on associations between scent cues from other volatile substances in food and on the nutritional results of eating the foods (Bartoshuk, 1991; Schiffman et al., 1999). We experience warm foods as sweeter, but temperature does not alter our experience of saltiness (Cruz & Green, 2000). Warming releases aromas that rise from the mouth into the nose and create more flavor sensations. This is why some people find hot pizza delicious and cold pizza disgusting. Spicy “hot” foods actually stimulate pain fibers in the mouth because they contain a substance called capsaicin (pronounced “kap-SAY-uh-sin”), which stimulates pain sensing neurons that are also stimulated by heat. Our Sense of Smell Pinching your nose prevents you from smelling odors, and a dilator strip on the bridge of your nose opens your nasal passages and intensifies odors (Raudenbush & Meyer, 2002). These effects occur because the nose (and the mouth, to some extent) acts as an accessory structure that collects airborne odor molecules for coding and analysis by the olfactory system (see Figure 3.16). As odor molecules pass into the moist lining of the upper part of the nose—called the mucous membrane—they bind to receptors on the dendrites of olfactory neurons, causing a biochemical change. This change, in turn, leads to changes in the firing rates of these neurons, whose axons combine to form the olfactory nerve (Dionne & Dubin, 1994). It takes only a single molecule of an odorous substance to cause a change in the activity of an olfactory neuron, but detection of the odor by a human requires about fifty such molecules (Menini, Picco, & Firestein, 1995). The number of molecules needed to trigger an olfactory sensation can vary, however. FIGURE 3.16 Olfactory bulb The Olfactory System: The Nose and the Rose Airborne odor molecules reach the olfactory area either through the nose or through an opening in the palate at the back of the mouth. This opening allows us to sample odors from our food as we eat. Nerve fibers pass directly from the olfactory area to the olfactory bulb in the brain, and from there signals pass to areas that are involved in emotion. This arrangement helps explain why odors often trigger strong emotional memories. Receptor cells Olfactory bulb Olfactory area Tongue 104 olfactory bulb A brain structure that receives messages regarding smell. pheromones Chemicals that are released by one creature and detected by another, shaping the second one’s behavior or physiology. Chapter 3 Sensation and Perception For example, women are more sensitive to odors during certain phases of their menstrual cycles (Navarrete-Palacios et al., 2003). There are thousands of different receptors for odors, but there are even more possible odors in the world. Any particular odor is sensed as a particular pattern of responses by these odorant receptors (Kajiya et al., 2001; Zou & Buck, 2006). So a rose, a pizza, and your favorite cologne each have a different smell because they stimulate their own unique patterns of activity in your odorant receptors. The question of how smells are coded has been of special interest since the September 11, 2001, terrorist attacks in the United States. Researchers have intensified their efforts to develop an “electronic nose” capable of detecting odorants associated with guns and explosives (Thaler, Kennedy, & Hanson, 2001). Versions of these devices are already in use at some airports. Unlike other senses, our sense of smell does not send its messages through the thalamus. Instead, axons from olfactory neurons in the nose extend through a bony plate and directly into the brain, where they have a synapse in a structure called the olfactory bulb. Connections from the olfactory bulb spread throughout the brain (Zou, Li, & Buck, 2005), but they are especially plentiful in the amygdala, a part of the brain involved in emotional experience and learning. In humans, the amygdala is especially active in response to disgusting odors (Zald & Pardo, 1997). The unique anatomy of the olfactory system may help account for the unique relationship between smells and emotion (Stevenson & Boakes, 2003). Associations between particular odors and experiences—especially emotional experiences—are not weakened much by time or later experiences (Lawless & Engen, 1977). So catching a whiff of the cologne once worn by a lost loved one can reactivate intense feelings of love or sadness associated with that person. Odors can also bring back accurate memories of experiences linked with them, especially positive experiences (Engen, Gilmore, & Mair, 1991; Mohr et al., 2001). Species ranging from humans to worms have remarkably similar neural mechanisms for sensing smell. And all mammals, including humans, have brain systems for detecting the source of smells by comparing the strength of sensory inputs reaching the left and right nostrils (Porter et al., 2005). Different species vary considerably, however, in their sensitivity to odor and in the degree to which they depend on it for survival. Humans have about 9 million olfactory neurons, compared with about 225 million in dogs, a species that is far more dependent on smell to identify food, territory, and receptive mates. Dogs and many other species also have an accessory olfactory system that detects pheromones. Pheromones (pronounced “FAIR-oh-mohns”) are chemicals that, when released by one creature and detected by another, can shape the second animal’s behavior or physiology (Silvotti, Montanu, & Tirindelli, 2003). For example, when a male snake detects a chemical on the skin of a female snake, it is stimulated to “court” the female. The role of pheromones in humans is much less clear, but it appears that we do have some sort of pheromone-like system (Berglund, Lindström, Savic, 2006; Savic, Berglund, & Lindström, 2005). A possible human gene for pheromone receptors has been found (Rodriguez et al., 2000), and pheromones have been shown to cause reproduction-related physiological changes in humans (Grammer, Fink, & Neave, 2005). Specifically, pheromonal signals secreted in women’s perspiration can influence nearby women’s menstrual cycles. As a result, women living together eventually tend to menstruate at about the same time (Stern & McClintock, 1998). Furthermore, odorants that cannot be consciously detected can nevertheless influence mood and stimulate activity in nonolfactory areas of the brain (Jacob & McClintock, 2000; Jacob et al., 2001; Savic et al., 2001). Despite steamy ads for cologne and perfume, however, there is not yet any solid evidence that humans give off or can detect pheromones that act as sexual attractants. If a certain scent does enhance a person’s readiness for sex, it is probably because the person has learned to associate that scent with previous sexual experiences. There are many other examples of people using olfactory information in social situations. For instance, after just a few hours of contact with their newborn babies, mothers can usually identify them by the infants’ smell (Porter, Cernich, & McLaughlin, 1983). And if