Comments
Description
Transcript
Obstacles to Problem Solving
259 Problem Solving ■ What did the researcher find? Bradshaw found several factors that might have contributed to the Wright brothers’ success. First, as bachelors, they had a lot of spare time to work on their designs. Second, they owned a bicycle shop, so they were familiar with lightweight, but sturdy, structures. Third, they were brothers who had a good working relationship. And finally, as mechanics, they were good with their hands. Were any of these features directly responsible for their successful invention of the airplane? Perhaps, but Bradshaw’s use of the comparative case study method revealed that everyone else working on the problem of flight shared at least one of these features with the Wright brothers. For instance, an engineer named Octave Chanute was good with his hands and familiar with lightweight, sturdy structures. And two other pairs of brothers had worked together to try to invent a flying machine. However, Bradshaw found one feature that was unique to the Wright brothers’ approach. Of all the inventors working on the problem, only the Wrights spent considerable time and energy testing aircraft components before field-testing complete machines. This feature was important because even the best designs of the day flew for only a few seconds—far too briefly to reveal what was working and what was not. As a result, inventors had to guess about what to fix and often ended up with an “improved” model that was worse than the previous one. ■ What do the results mean? Bradshaw’s comparative case study method suggested that the problem-solving strategy of decomposition was the basis for the Wright brothers’ success. By testing components, they were able to collect the information they needed to develop an efficient propeller, improve the shape of the wings for maximum lift, and refine other vital components of their aircraft. ■ What do we still need to know? Decomposition is a strategy often seen in the laboratory, and as demonstrated by the case of the Wright brothers, it is a potentially important aspect of major inventions and discoveries beyond the laboratory. But is decomposition, or means-end analysis, used in other real-world settings as well? To find out, researchers will need to conduct additional studies of people’s mental strategies as they attempt to solve problems ranging from how to install a new computer to how to efficiently search the Internet. Obstacles to Problem Solving The failure of the Wright brothers’ competitors to use decomposition is just one example of the obstacles that face problem solvers every day. Difficulties frequently occur at the start, during the diagnosis stage, when a person forms and then tests hypotheses about a problem. As a case in point, consider this true story: In September 1998, John Gatiss was in the kitchen of his rented house in Cheltenham, England, when he heard a faint “meowing” sound. Worried that a kitten had become trapped somewhere, he called for the fire brigade to rescue the animal. The sound seemed to be coming from the electric stove, so the rescuers dismantled it, disconnecting the power cord in the process. The sound stopped, but everyone assumed that wherever the kitten was, it was now too frightened to meow. The search was reluctantly abandoned, and the stove was reconnected; four days later, however, the meowing began anew. This time, Gatiss and his landlord called the Royal Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals (RSPCA), whose inspectors heard the kitten in distress and asked the fire brigade 260 FIGURE Chapter 7 Thought, Language, and Intelligence 7.5 The Jar Problem The task here is to come up with the number of quarts of by water shown in the first column by using jars with the capacities shown in the next three columns. Each line represents a different problem, and you have an unlimited supply of water for each one. Try to solve all seven problems without looking at the answers in the text. doing 2 learn Quantity 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 21 10 19 21 18 6 15 quarts quarts quarts quarts quarts quarts quarts Jar A 8 6 5 20 8 7 12 Jar B 35 18 32 57 40 17 33 Jar C 3 1 4 8 7 2 3 to come back. They spent the next three days searching for the cat. First, they dismantled parts of the kitchen walls and ripped up the floorboards. Next, they called in plumbing and drainage specialists, who used cables tipped with fiber-optic cameras to search remote cavities where a kitten might hide. Rescuers then brought in a disaster search team, which tried to find the kitten with acoustic and ultrasonic equipment normally used to locate victims trapped under earthquake debris. Not a sound was heard. Increasingly concerned about how much longer the kitten could survive, the fire brigade tried to coax it from hiding with the finest-quality fish, but to no avail. Suddenly, there was a burst of “purring” that, to everyone’s surprise (and the landlord’s dismay), was traced by the ultrasonic equipment to the clock in the electric stove! Later, the landlord commented that everyone assumed Gatiss’s original hypothesis was correct—that the “meowing” came from a cat trapped in the kitchen. “I just let them carry on. If there is an animal in there, you have to do what it takes. The funniest thing was that it seemed to reply when we called out to it” (London Daily Telegraph, 1998). How could fifteen fire-rescue workers, three RSPCA inspectors, four drainage workers, and two acoustics experts waste eight days and cause nearly $2,000 in damage to a house in pursuit of a nonexistent kitten? The answer lies in the fact that they, like the rest of us, were prone to four main obstacles to efficient problem solving, described in the following sections. mental set The tendency for old patterns of problem solving to persist. functional fixedness The tendency to think about familiar objects in familiar ways. Multiple Hypotheses Often, we begin to solve a problem with only a hazy notion of which hypotheses to test. Suppose you heard a strange sound in your kitchen. It could be caused by several different things, but which hypotheses should you test, and in what order? People have a difficult time working with more than two or three hypotheses at a time (Mehle, 1982). The limited capacity of short-term memory may be part of the reason (Halford et al., 2005). As discussed in the memory chapter, a person can hold only about seven chunks of information in short-term memory. Because a single hypothesis, let alone two or three, might include more than seven chunks, it may be difficult or impossible to keep them all in mind at once. Further, the availability and representativeness heuristics may lead people to choose the hypothesis that comes most easily to mind and seems most likely to fit the circumstances (Tversky & Kahneman, 1974). That hypothesis may be wrong, though, meaning that the correct hypothesis is never considered. Mr. Gatiss diagnosed what he heard as distressed meowing because it sounded more like a kitten than a clock and because it was easier to imagine an animal trapped behind the stove than a suddenly faulty clock inside it. 261 Problem Solving FIGURE 7.6 The Nine-Dot Problem The problem is to draw no more than four straight lines by that run through all nine dots on the page without lifting your pencil from the paper. Figure 7.8 shows two ways of going beyond mental sets to solve this problem. doing 2 learn Mental Sets Sometimes people are so blinded by one hypothesis or strategy that they stick with it even when better alternatives should be obvious. This is a clear case of the anchoring heuristic at work. Once Gatiss reported hearing a “trapped kitten,” his description created an assumption that everyone else accepted and that no one challenged. Figure 7.5 shows a problem-solving situation in which such errors often appear. The first problem in the figure is to come up with 21 quarts of liquid by learn using 3 jars that have capacities of 8, 35, and 3 quarts, respectively. Before by doing you read any further, try to solve this problem and all the others listed in Figure 7.5. How did you do? You probably figured out that the solution to the first problem is to fill Jar B to its capacity of 35 quarts, and then use its contents to fill Jar A to its capacity of 8 quarts, leaving 27 quarts in Jar B. Finally, you pour from Jar B to fill Jar C twice, leaving 21 quarts in Jar B [27 (2 3) = 21]. You probably found that a similar solution worked for each problem. In fact, by the time you reached Problem 7, you might have developed a mental set, a tendency for old patterns of problem solving to persist (Luchins, 1942; Sweller & Gee, 1978). If so, your mental set probably caused you to use the same old formula (B A 2C) even though a simpler one (A C) would have worked just as well. Figures 7.6 and 7.8 show another way in which mental sets can restrict our perception of the possible solutions to a problem. Another restriction on problem solving may come from experience with objects. Once people become familiar with using an object for one purpose, they may be blinded to other ways of using it. Long experience may produce functional fixedness, a tendency to use familiar objects in familiar, rather than creative, ways (German & Barrett, 2005). Figure 7.7 provides an example. An incubation strategy often helps to break mental sets. 2 Ignoring Negative Evidence On September 26, 1983, Lt. Col. Stanislav Petrov was in command of a secret facility that analyzed information from Russian early-warning satellites. Suddenly, alarms went off as computers found evidence of five U.S. missiles being launched toward Russia. Tension between the two countries was high at the time, so, based on the availability heuristic, Petrov hypothesized that a nuclear attack was FIGURE 7.7 An Example of Functional Fixedness Before reading further, look at this drawing and ask yourself how you would fasten together two strings that are hanging from the ceiling but are out of reach of each other. Several tools are available, yet most people don’t think of attaching, say, a pair of pliers to one string and swinging it like a pendulum until it can be reached while holding the other string. This solution is not obvious because we tend to fixate on the function of pliers as a tool rather than as a weight. People are more likely to solve this problem if the tools are scattered around the room. When the pliers are in a toolbox, their function as a tool is emphasized, and functional fixedness becomes nearly impossible to break. doing 2 learn by